Wine-tasting:
it's junk science
Experiments
have shown that people can't tell plonk from grand cru. Now one US winemaker
claims that even experts can't judge wine accurately. What's the science behind
the taste?
Source:
The Observer
David
Derbyshire
Saturday
22 June 2013
Every
year Robert Hodgson selects the finest wines from his small California winery
and puts them into competitions around the state.
And in
most years, the results are surprisingly inconsistent: some whites rated as
gold medallists in one contest do badly in another. Reds adored by some panels
are dismissed by others. Over the decades Hodgson, a softly spoken retired
oceanographer, became curious. Judging wines is by its nature subjective, but
the awards appeared to be handed out at random.
So
drawing on his background in statistics, Hodgson approached the organisers of
the California State Fair wine competition, the oldest contest of its kind in
North America, and proposed an experiment for their annual June tasting
sessions.
Each
panel of four judges would be presented with their usual "flight" of
samples to sniff, sip and slurp. But some wines would be presented to the panel
three times, poured from the same bottle each time. The results would be
compiled and analysed to see whether wine testing really is scientific.
The
first experiment took place in 2005. The last was in Sacramento earlier this
month. Hodgson's findings have stunned the wine industry. Over the years he has
shown again and again that even trained, professional palates are terrible at
judging wine.
"The
results are disturbing," says Hodgson from the Fieldbrook Winery in
Humboldt County, described by its owner as a rural paradise. "Only about
10% of judges are consistent and those judges who were consistent one year were
ordinary the next year.
"Chance
has a great deal to do with the awards that wines win."
These
judges are not amateurs either. They read like a who's who of the American wine
industry from winemakers, sommeliers, critics and buyers to wine consultants
and academics. In Hodgson's tests, judges rated wines on a scale running from
50 to 100. In practice, most wines scored in the 70s, 80s and low 90s.
Results
from the first four years of the experiment, published in the Journal of Wine
Economics, showed a typical judge's scores varied by plus or minus four points
over the three blind tastings. A wine deemed to be a good 90 would be rated as
an acceptable 86 by the same judge minutes later and then an excellent 94.
Some of
the judges were far worse, others better - with around one in 10 varying their scores
by just plus or minus two. A few points may not sound much but it is enough to
swing a contest - and gold medals are worth a significant amount in extra sales
for wineries.
Hodgson
went on to analyse the results of wine competitions across California, and
found that their medals were distributed at random.
"I
think there are individual expert tasters with exceptional abilities sitting
alone who have a good sense, but when you sit 100 wines in front of them the
task is beyond human ability," he says. "We have won our fair share
of gold medals but now I have to say we were lucky."
His
studies have irritated many figures in the industry. "They say I'm full of
bullshit but that's OK. I'm proud of what I do. It's part of my academic
background to find the truth.''
Hodgson
isn't alone in questioning the science of wine-tasting. French academic
Frédéric Brochet tested the effect of labels in 2001. He presented the same
Bordeaux superior wine to 57 volunteers a week apart and in two different
bottles - one for a table wine, the other for a grand cru.
The
tasters were fooled.
When
tasting a supposedly superior wine, their language was more positive -
describing it as complex, balanced, long and woody. When the same wine was
presented as plonk, the critics were more likely to use negatives such as weak,
light and flat.
In 2008
a study of 6,000 blind tastings by Robin Goldstein in the Journal of Wine
Economics found a positive link between the price of wine and the amount people
enjoyed it. But the link only existed for people trained to detect the elements
of wine that make them expensive.
In 2011
Professor Richard Wiseman, a psychologist (and former professional magician) at
Hertfordshire University invited 578 people to comment on a range of red and
white wines, varying from £3.49 for a claret to £30 for champagne, and tasted
blind.
People
could tell the difference between wines under £5 and those above £10 only 53%
of the time for whites and only 47% of the time for reds. Overall they would
have been just as a successful flipping a coin to guess.
So why
are ordinary drinkers and the experts so poor at tasting blind? Part of the
answer lies in the sheer complexity of wine.
For a
drink made by fermenting fruit juice, wine is a remarkably sophisticated chemical
cocktail. Dr Bryce Rankine, an Australian wine scientist, identified 27
distinct organic acids in wine, 23 varieties of alcohol in addition to the
common ethanol, more than 80 esters and aldehydes, 16 sugars, plus a long list
of assorted vitamins and minerals that wouldn't look out of place on the
ingredients list of a cereal pack. There are even harmless traces of lead and
arsenic that come from the soil.
Three
of wine's most basic qualities - sweetness, sourness and bitterness - are
picked up by the tongue's taste buds. A good wine has the perfect balance of
sweet from the sugar in grapes, sourness from the acids, particularly tartaric
and malic acid, and bitterness from alcohol and polyphenols, including tannins.
Many
wines are more acidic than lemon juice and are only palatable because that
acidity is balanced by sweetness and bitterness. "It's the holy trinity of
the palate - sugar, acid and alcohol," says Dr James Hutchinson, a wine
expert at the Royal Society of Chemistry.
Professionals
distinguish between the balance of these three basic elements and a wine's
flavour. And here the chemistry gets more complicated.
The
flavour of wine - its aroma or bouquet - is detected not by the taste buds, but
by millions of receptors in the olfactory bulb, a blob of nervous tissue where
the brain meets the nasal passage.
Chemists
have identified at least 400 aroma compounds that work on their own and with
others to create complex flavours - some appearing immediately on first
sniffing, others emerging only as an aftertaste. Most of these are volatiles -
aromatic compounds that tend to have a low boiling point and waft away from
glasses and tongues towards the olfactory bulb.
Some of
these, the primary volatiles, are present in the grape. Others, the
secondaries, are generated by yeast activity during fermentation. The rest, the
tertiary volatiles, are formed as wine matures in barrels or bottles.
Over
the last few decades, wine scientists have begun to identify the compounds
responsible for some of the distinctive aromas in wine.
The
grassy, gooseberry quality of sauvignon blanc, for instance, comes from a class
of chemicals called methoxypyrazines. These contain nitrogen and are byproducts
of the metabolism of amino acids in the grape. Concentrations are higher in
cooler climates, which is why New Zealand sauvignon blancs are often more
herbaceous than Australian ones.
The
flowery aroma of muscat and gewürztraminer comes from a class of alcohol
compounds called monoterpenes. These include linalool - a substance also used
in perfumes and insecticide - and geraniol, a pale yellow liquid that doubles
up as an effective mosquito repellent and gives geranium its distinctive smell.
The
spicy notes of chardonnay have been attributed to compounds called
megastigmatrienones, also found in grapefruit juice.
"People
underestimate how clever the olfactory system is at detecting aromas and our
brain is at interpreting them," says Hutchinson.
"The
olfactory system has the complexity in terms of its protein receptors to detect
all the different aromas, but the brain response isn't always up to it. But I'm
a believer that everyone has the same equipment and it comes down to learning
how to interpret it." Within eight tastings, most people can learn to
detect and name a reasonable range of aromas in wine, Hutchinson says.
Detecting
and finding the right vocabulary may be within everyone's grasp. But when it
comes to ranking wines, Hutchinson shares Robert Hodgson's concerns.
"There's
a lot of nonsense and emperor's new clothes in the wine world," Hutchinson
says. "I have had a number of wines costing hundreds of pounds that have
disappointed me - and a number costing between £5 and £10 which have been
absolutely surprising."
People
struggle with assessing wine because the brain's interpretation of aroma and
bouquet is based on far more than the chemicals found in the drink. Temperature
plays a big part. Volatiles in wine are more active when wine is warmer. Serve
a New World chardonnay too cold and you'll only taste the overpowering oak.
Serve a red too warm and the heady boozy qualities will be overpowering.
Colour
affects our perceptions too. In 2001 Frédérick Brochet of the University of
Bordeaux asked 54 wine experts to test two glasses of wine - one red, one
white. Using the typical language of tasters, the panel described the red as
"jammy' and commented on its crushed red fruit.
The
critics failed to spot that both wines were from the same bottle. The only difference
was that one had been coloured red with a flavourless dye.
Other
environmental factors play a role. A judge's palate is affected by what she or
he had earlier, the time of day, their tiredness, their health - even the
weather.
For
Hutchinson and Hodgson the unpredictability means that human scores of wines
are of limited value.
So if
people cannot be relied on to judge wine, how about machines?
"In
terms of replicating what a human can do we are a long way off,"
Hutchinson says. "The one thing we can do well, though, is a lot of
amazing analytical chemistry that allows us to detect a huge range of different
compounds in a glass of wine.
''We
can start to have an indication of how the acidity balances with the sweetness
and different levels of flavour compounds.
"But
the step we haven't got to is how that raw chemical information can be crunched
together and converted into something that reflects someone's emotional
response. That might be something we can never achieve."
Meanwhile
the blind tasting contests go on. Robert Hodgson is determined to improve the
quality of judging. He has developed a test that will determine whether a
judge's assessment of a blind-tasted glass in a medal competition is better
than chance. The research will be presented at a conference in Cape Town this
year. But the early findings are not promising.
"So
far I've yet to find someone who passes," he says.
PUNGENT
OVERTONES
In
2007, Richard E Quandt, a Princeton economics professor, published a paper
entitled "On Wine Bullshit: Some New Software?" The study sought to
describe the "unholy union" of "bullshit and bullshit artists
who are impelled to comment on it", in this case wine and wine critics.
Quandt compiled a "vocabulary of wine descriptors" containing 123 terms
from "angular" to "violets" via other nonsense descriptions
such as "fireplace" and "tannins, fine-grained".
Then,
with the help of colleagues, he built an algorithm that generated wine reviews
of hypothetical wines using his "vocabulary of bullshit". For
instance: "Château L'Ordure Pomerol, 2004. Fine minerality, dried apricots
and cedar characterise this sage-laden wine bursting with black fruit and
toasty oak." He concluded that whether his reviews were "any more
bullshit" than real ones was a "judgment call". Sadly, he didn't
explore how long it would take a monkey to type a wine review.